General Health

General health issues, Medical conditions, Research and studies and more

Mental Health

Natural Medicine

Nutritional supplements, Herbs, Alternative medicine and more…

Wellness & Lifestyle

Nutrition, Diets, Healthy living, Detox, Exercise and Physical Fitness, Sports Fitness and more…

Women’s Health

Relationships, Pregnancy, Birth control, Menopause and more

Home » Information, News

Brain imaging study shows physiological basis of dyslexia

Article / Review by on September 28, 2011 – 9:37 pmNo Comments

Brain imaging study shows physiological basis of dyslexia

Researchers at the Stanford University School of Medicine have used an imaging technique to show that the brain activation patterns in children with poor reading skills and a low IQ are similar to those in poor readers with a typical IQ. The work provides more definitive evidence about poor readers having similar kinds of difficulties regardless of their general cognitive ability. 

Schools and psychologists have historically relied on a child’s IQ to define and diagnose dyslexia, a brain-based learning disability that impairs a person’s ability to read: If a child’s reading achievement was below expectation based on IQ, he would be considered dyslexic, while a poor reader with a low IQ would receive some other diagnosis. But these new findings provide “biological evidence that IQ should not be emphasized in the diagnosis of reading abilities,” said Fumiko Hoeft, MD, PhD, an instructor at Stanford’s Center for Interdisciplinary Brain Sciences Research, who is senior author of the study, which will appear in an upcoming issue of Psychological Science.

Fumiko Hoeft, MD, PhD, an instructor at Stanford’s Center for Interdisciplinary Brain Sciences Research (CIBSR)
Fumiko Hoeft, MD, PhD, an instructor at Stanford’s Center for Interdisciplinary Brain Sciences Research (CIBSR)

The new results come in the wake of recent behavioral studies showing that phonological deficits — that is, difficulties in processing the sound system of language, which often leads to difficulties in connecting the sounds of language to letters — are similar in poor readers regardless of IQ. Indeed, the 2004 reauthorization of the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act mandated that states no longer require school districts to use IQ tests in identifying individuals with learning disabilities such as dyslexia.

“There’s a disassociation between what is established in research and what is happening in practice,” said Hoeft, explaining that many U.S. schools still rely on a discrepancy between reading achievement and IQ to define and diagnose dyslexia. At first glance, she added, it would seem to make sense that poor readers with typical IQs would have different learning challenges than those with low ones.

The use of IQ in diagnosing dyslexia, which affects 5 to 17 percent of U.S. children, has real implications for poor readers. If children aren’t diagnosed as dyslexic, they don’t qualify for services that a typical dyslexic does, and they’re not taught strategies to overcome specific problems in the way they view and process words.

To further understand what happens in the brains of poor readers with different IQs, Hoeft turned to imaging. She and her colleagues expected poor readers with typical IQs to exhibit similar patterns of brain activation as poor readers with low IQs. Their experiments, she said, were intended to confirm that the two groups had the same neurophysiological basis for impaired phonological processing and that their reading problems were not related to IQ.

The study involved 131 children, ranging from 7 to 16 years old, from Allegheny County, Penn., and the San Francisco Bay Area. The children were put into three groups: poor readers with typical IQ, poor readers with low IQ and typical readers with typical IQ. The children then took a reading test and underwent a brain-imaging technique called functional magnetic resonance imaging, or fMRI, as they completed a task that involved judging whether two visually presented words rhymed (e.g., bait and gate) or not (e.g., price or miss).

In both samples, the typical readers had significantly higher reading-related scores and more accurate performance on the rhyme-judgment task than the two other groups. And there were no significant differences between the two groups of poor readers on these measures.

In the fMRI analysis, researchers found that both groups of poor readers exhibited significantly reduced activations relative to typical readers in the left inferior parietal lobule and left fusiform gyrus. The researchers also used a sophisticated analysis to determine that the brain patterns of each group of poor readers looked liked those of the other group of poor readers more than 80 percent of the time, and did not often resemble the patterns from the normal readers.

Hoeft noted that the results are timely. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, the standard diagnostic guide for mental illnesses and brain disorders, is currently being revised, and there is a proposal to change it so that IQ wouldn’t be taken into consideration when diagnosing dyslexia. (The new version, DSM V, will be released in 2013.) This work, she said, is the “first study reporting biological neuroimaging evidence to support” that change.

“Convergent psychological, educational and now neurobiological evidence suggests that the long-standing and widely applied diagnosis of dyslexia by IQ discrepancy is not supported,” the researchers wrote in the paper.

Hoeft and her colleagues also point out that these and other findings indicate that “any child with a reading difficulty, regardless of his or her general level of cognitive abilities (IQ), should be encouraged to seek reading intervention.”

Hoeft said she will continue her work in this area and is hoping to use imaging to predict outcomes of poor readers. She also plans to look at younger readers to see if imaging can be used to diagnose children at younger ages.

The study’s two lead authors are Stanford graduate student Hiroko Tanaka and Jessica Black, PhD, of Boston College. The other Stanford co-authors are graduate student Leanne Stanley; Shelli Kesler, PhD, assistant professor of psychiatry and behavioral sciences; and Allan Reiss, MD, the Howard C. Robbins Professor of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, a professor of radiology and the director of Stanford’s Center for Interdisciplinary Brain Sciences Research. Researchers from the Massachusetts Institute of Technology are also co-authors.

The work was supported by the William and Flora Hewlett Foundation, Richard King Mellon Foundation, Ellison Medical Foundation, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, Lucile Packard Foundation for Children’s Health, Spectrum Child Health & Clinical and Translational Science Award, Dyslexia Foundation and the National Alliance for Research in Schizophrenia and Depression.

 …

Fumiko Hoeft, MD, PhD. Bio.
Fumiko Hoeft
Academic Appointments
Instructor, Psychiatry & Behavioral Science – Interdis Brain Science Research

> Administrative Appointments
Positions and Employment
– 2003 – 2007 Visiting Scientist, Division of Biology, California Institute of Technology, CA USA
– 2005 – 2008 Research Associate (’05-‘06), Senior Research Scientist (’06-’08), CIBSR, Stanford University School of Medicine, CA USA
– 2008 – 2011 Instructor (5/1-), Associate Director of Neuroimaging Applications, Center for Interdisciplinary Brain Sciences Research (CIBSR), Stanford University School of Medicine, CA USA
* – 2012 – Fumiko Hoeft is moving to UCSF as an Associate Professor of Psychiatry (on Jan 1, 2012)

> Current Research Interests
Developmental and Cognitive Neurosciences. Pediatric clinical neuroimaging. Dr. Fumiko Hoeft is interested in figuring out ways that cognitive neuroscience research can inform educational and clinical practices. Examination of brain development in children. Developing practical (clinical and educational) applications of neuroimaging: e.g. 1. Using machine learning methods and neuroimaging, behavioral, cognitive, genetic and environmental measures to predict outcome (interventions, academic, clinical), and 2. cognitive training using real-time fMRI (fNIRS) feedback. Learning disabilities such as developmental dyslexia, neurodevelopmental disorders such as autism, fragile X syndrome, Klinefelter syndrome and Williams syndrome.

> Education/ Training 
Stanford University, Dept Psychology, CA USA Postdoc 2003-2005  Cognitive Neuroscience
Keio Univ Sch of Med, Tokyo, Japan Ph.D. 2003 Neuropsychiatry, Clinical Neuroscience
California Institute of Technology, Div of Biology, CA Research Fellow 2000-2003 System Neuroscience
UCLA Sch of Med, Brain Mapping Center, CA Research Fellow 2000-2002 Cognitive Neuroscience
Harvard Med Sch, BIDMC, Dept Neurology, MA Research Fellow 1998-2000 Neurophysiology
Keio Univ Sch of Med, Tokyo, Japan Residency 1995-1997 Neuropsychiatry, Clin Neurophys
Keio Univ Sch of Med, Tokyo, Japan M.D. 1995 Medicine

> Honors and Awards
K23, NICHD (’08-’13)
Young Investigator Award, NARSAD (’08-’10)
Pediatric Research Fund Award – Pilot Early Career, CHRP, Stanford (’08-’10)
Pediatric Research Fund Award – Pilot Early Career, CHRP, Stanford (’07-’09)
Research Award in Consciousness, Mind Science Foundation (’05-’06)
Award for Outstanding Contribution, World Psychiatric Association (WPA) (’02)
Fellowship Award, Biological Psychiatry (’01)
Trainee Award, OHBM (’00)

 About Center for Interdisciplinary Brain Sciences Research (CIBSR) at the Stanford University School of Medicine/ Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences

About Center for Interdisciplinary Brain Sciences Research (CIBSR) at the Stanford University School of Medicine/ Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences

The Center for Interdisciplinary Brain Sciences Research (CIBSR) at the Stanford University School of Medicine is dedicated to research that will improve the lives and well-being of individuals with disorders of the brain. In particular, CIBSR research staff are dedicated to identifying biological and environmental risk factors, understanding disease pathophysiology and developmental outcomes, and developing new treatments for neurodevelopmental, neurogenetic and neuropsychiatric disorders of childhood onset. The research studies are truly multi/interdisciplinary as they bring together experts from the fields of psychiatry, neurology, psychology, computer science, biostatistics and genetics to explore and seek answers for complex questions related to brain-behavior relationships.

Active research at CIBSR includes:
> Mutlimodal imaging of the brain utilizing anatomical and functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS).
> Behavioral, cognitive, and physiological assessment to address questions concerning the influence of biological and environmental factors on outcome.
> The development of new biological and cognitive-behavioral treatments.
> Development of brain image analysis methods and software.

* Information about the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, which also supported the research, is available at http://psychiatry.stanford.edu/
**  The above story is adapted from materials provided by Stanford University School of Medicine 

________________________________________________________________

Stanford school of Medicine logo

Tags: , , , , , , , , , , , ,

Leave a comment!

Add your comment below, or trackback from your own site. You can also subscribe to these comments via RSS.

Be nice. Keep it clean. Stay on topic. No spam.

You can use these tags:
<a href="" title=""> <abbr title=""> <acronym title=""> <b> <blockquote cite=""> <cite> <code> <del datetime=""> <em> <i> <q cite=""> <s> <strike> <strong>